L’inconscio. Perché ci è voluto così tanto tempo, cosa sappiamo oggi, e alcune implicazioni cliniche

Titolo Rivista PSICOTERAPIA E SCIENZE UMANE
Autori/Curatori Joel Weinberger
Anno di pubblicazione 2025 Fascicolo 2025/3
Lingua Italiano Numero pagine 34 P. 377-410 Dimensione file 347 KB
DOI 10.3280/PU2025-003001
Il DOI è il codice a barre della proprietà intellettuale: per saperne di più clicca qui

Qui sotto puoi vedere in anteprima la prima pagina di questo articolo.

Se questo articolo ti interessa, lo puoi acquistare (e scaricare in formato pdf) seguendo le facili indicazioni per acquistare il download credit. Acquista Download Credits per scaricare questo Articolo in formato PDF

Anteprima articolo

FrancoAngeli è membro della Publishers International Linking Association, Inc (PILA)associazione indipendente e non profit per facilitare (attraverso i servizi tecnologici implementati da CrossRef.org) l’accesso degli studiosi ai contenuti digitali nelle pubblicazioni professionali e scientifiche

Vengono esaminati i processi inconsci da prospettive storiche, empiriche, neurocognitive e cliniche. Lo studio scientifico dei processi inconsci ha subìto un ritardo nella psicologia accademica a causa dell’identificazione cartesiana della mente col conscio. Questo fenomeno è diventato ancora più estremo quando il comportamentismo ha completamente ignorato i processi mentali. La psicoanalisi invece ha sempre dato importanza all’inconscio, ma il suo isolamento dalla psicologia accademica e l’avversione per la ricerca empirico-sperimentale ne hanno limitato l’influenza. Il mondo accademico, a sua volta, ha ignorato le intuizioni psicoanalitiche, e alla fine è arrivato a riconoscere i processi inconsci, considerandoli normali, fisiologici. Vengono esaminati alcuni dei principali risultati della ricerca e vengono suggerite alcune implicazioni cliniche. Infine, vengono esaminati tre modelli neurocognitivi della mente come possibili metapsicologie. Tutti postulano l’elaborazione inconscia, parallela e associativa, e convalidano molti concetti psicodinamici tra cui quelli di incoerenza, conflitto e compromesso, ma i processi inconsci non sono visti necessariamente come patologici: sono normali e favoriscono l’adattamento, tuttavia a volte si rivelano controproducenti.

Parole chiave:Inconscio; Modelli neurocognitivi della mente; Ricerca sui processi inconsci; Psicoanalisi; Implicazioni cliniche

  1. Alpers G.W. & Gerdes A.B. (2007). Here is looking at you: Emotional faces predominate in binocular rivalry. Emotion, 7, 3: 495-506. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.495
  2. Alpers G.W. & Pauli P. (2006). Emotional pictures predominate in binocular rivalry. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 5: 596-607. DOI: 10.1080/02699930500282249
  3. Anderson J.R. (2007). How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe? Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  4. Anderson M.L. (2008). Circuit sharing and the implementation of intelligent systems. Connection Science, 20, 4: 239-251. DOI: 10.1080/09540090802413202
  5. Anderson M.L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 4: 245-313. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X10000853
  6. Anderson M.L., Kinnison J. & Pessoa L. (2013). A network of topologically organized, functionally diverse brain regions supporting cognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 10: 1529-1545.
  7. Anderson M.L. (2014). After Phrenology: Neural Reuse and the Interactive Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  8. Anderson M.L., Kinnison J. & Pessoa L. (2013). Describing functional diversity of brain regions and brain networks. Neuroimage, 73: 50-58.
  9. Anderson M.L., Brumbaugh J. & Suben A. (2010). Investigating functional cooperation in the human brain using simple graph-theoretic methods. In: Chaovalitwongse W., Pardola P., & Xanthopoulos, editors, Computational Neuroscience. New York: Springer, 2010, pp. 31-42.
  10. Araujo S.F. (2012). Why did Wundt abandon his early theory of the unconscious? Wundt’s psychological project. History of Psychology, 15, 1: 33-49.
  11. Baddeley A.D. & Hitch G.J. (1974). Working memory. In: Bower G.A., editor, Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation, Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press, 1974, pp. 47-90.
  12. Bandura A. (1969). Principles of Behavior Modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  13. Bannerman R.L., Milders M., De Gelder B. & Sahraie A. (2008). Influence of emotional facial expressions on binocular rivalry. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 28, 4: 317-326.
  14. Bargh J.A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions necessary for nonconscious processing effects. American Journal of Psychology, 105, 2: 181-199.
  15. Bargh J.A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In: Wyer R.S. Jr. & Srull T.K., editors, Handbook of Social Cognition. Second Edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1994, pp. 1-40.
  16. Bargh J.A., Schwader K.L., Hailey S.E., Dyer R.L. & Boone A. (2012). Automaticity in social cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 12: 593-605.
  17. Baumeister R.F., Bratslavsky E., Finkenauer C. & Vohs K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 4: 323-370. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
  18. Beck A.T., Rush A.J., Shaw B.F. & Emery G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York: Guilford (trad. it.: Terapia cognitiva della depressione. Torino: Bolati Boringhieri, 1987).
  19. Berry D.C. & Dienes Z. (1993). Implicit Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  20. Bornstein R.F., & Masling J.M., editors (1998). Empirical Perspectives on the Psychoanalytic Unconscious. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
  21. Bornstein R.F. & Pittman T.S., editors (1992). Perception Without Awareness: Cognitive, Clinical, and Social Perspectives. New York: Guilford.
  22. Brenner C. (1982). The Mind in Conflict. New York: International Universities Press (trad. it.: La mente in conflitto. Firenze: Martinelli, 1985).
  23. Broadbent D.E. (1971). Decision and Stress. New York: Academic Press.
  24. Brody N. (1972). Personality: Research and Theory. New York: Academic Press.
  25. Calvo M.G., Avero P. & Nummenmaa L. (2011). Primacy of emotional vs. semantic scene recognition in peripheral vision. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 8: 1358-1375. DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2010.544448
  26. Campbell W.K. & Sedikides C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3, 1: 23-43. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23
  27. Carruthers P. (2006). The Architecture of the Mind: Massive Modularity and the Flexibility of Thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
  28. Chan K.Q., Tong E.M.W., Tan D.H. & Koh A.H.Q. (2013). What do love and jealousy taste like? Emotion, 13, 6: 1142-1149.
  29. Cherry E.C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25: 975-979. DOI: 10.1121/1.1907229
  30. Cherry E.C. & Taylor W.K. (1954). Some further experiments upon the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 26, 554-559. DOI: 10.1121/1.1907373
  31. Civitarese G. (2023). On Bion’s concept of truth in an extra-moral sense. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 104, 2: 233-247.
  32. Cleeremans A. (2014). Connecting conscious and unconscious processing. Cognitive Science, 38, 6: 1286-1315.
  33. Cleeremans A. & Dienes Z. (2008). Computational models of implicit learning. In: Sun R. editor, The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Modeling. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2008, pp. 396-421.
  34. Cusimano C. & Goodwin G.P. (2019). Lay beliefs about the controllability of everyday mental states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 10: 1701-1732.
  35. Cusimano C. & Goodwin G.P. (2020). People judge others to have more voluntary control over beliefs than they themselves do. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119, 5: 999-1029.
  36. Damasio A.R. & Tranel D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 90, 11: 4857-4960.
  37. Damasio H., Grabowski T.J., Tranel D., Hichwa R.D. & Damasio A.R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380, 6574: 499-505.
  38. de Gelder B., Vroomen J., Pourtois G. & Weiskrantz L. (1999). Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. NeuroReport, 10, 18: 3753-3759. DOI: 10.1097/00001756-199912160-00007
  39. Dehaene S. & Cohen L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, 2: 384-398.
  40. de Roten Y., Drapeau M. & Michel L. (2008). Are there positive emotions in short-term dynamic psychotherapy or is it all Freude-less? Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 18, 2: 207-221. DOI: 10.1037/1053-0479.18.2.207
  41. Cartesio R. (1649). Les passions de l’âme (trad. it.: Le passioni dell’anima. Lanciano [CH]: Carabba, 1928; Torino: UTET, 1951; Bari: Laterza, 1954; Milano: Bompiani, 2003).
  42. Cartesio R. (1641). Meditationes de prima philosophia (trad. it.: Meditazioni metafisiche. Milano: Athena, 1927; Milano: Mondadori, 1929).
  43. Deutsch J.A. & Deutsch D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 1: 80-90.
  44. Dixon N.F. (1971). Subliminal Perception: The Nature of a Controversy. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  45. Dixon N.F. (1981). Preconscious Processing. New York: Wiley.
  46. Ekman P., Levenson R. & Friesen W. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 4616: 1208-1210.
  47. Ellenberger H.F. (1970). The Discovery of the Unconscious. New York: Basic Books (trad. it.: La scoperta dell’inconscio. Storia della psichiatria dinamica. Torino: Boringhieri, 1972).
  48. Erdelyi M.H. (1974). A new look at the new look: Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Review, 81, 1: 1-25.
  49. Eysenck H. (1985). Decline and Fall of the Freudian Empire. New York: Viking (trad. it.: Declino e caduta dell’impero freudiano. Torino: ACI, 1979).
  50. Fazio R.H., Sanbonmatsu D.M., Powell M.C. & Kardes F.R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 2: 229-238. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229
  51. Fonagy P. & Target M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 4: 679-700. DOI: 10.1017/S0954579497001399
  52. Forgas J.P., editor (2000). Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  53. Freud S. (1915). Metapsicologia. L’inconscio. Opere, 8: 49-88. Torino: Boringhieri, 1976.
  54. Freud S. (1932 [1933]). Introduzione alla psicoanalisi (nuova serie di lezioni). Opere, 11: 117-284. Torino: Boringhieri, 1979.
  55. Gallese V. (2007). Before and below “theory of mind”: Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 362, 1480: 659-669.
  56. Gallese V. & Lakoff G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 3: 455-479. DOI: 10.1080/02643290442000310
  57. Gardner H. (1985). The Mind’s New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Basic Books.
  58. Gilbert D.T. & Malone P.S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 1: 21-38. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21
  59. Glenberg A.M. & Kaschak M.P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 3: 558-565. DOI: 10.3758/BF03196313
  60. Glenberg A.M., Brown M. & Levin J. (2007). Enhancing comprehension in small reading groups using a manipulation strategy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 3: 389-399.
  61. Goldiamond I. (1958). Indicators of perception: 1. Subliminal perception, subception, unconscious perception: An analysis in terms of psychophysical indicator methodology. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 6: 373-411.
  62. Grünbaum A. (1984). The Foundations of Psychoanalysis. A Philosophical Critique. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press (trad.it.: I fondamenti della psicoanalisi. Milano: Il Saggiatore, 1988).
  63. Grünbaum A. et al. (1986). Reflections on “The Foundations of Psychoanalysis”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 2: 217-284 (trad. it. in: Pera M., a cura di, Psicoanalisi: obiezioni e risposte. Roma: Armando, 1988).
  64. Hassin R.R. (2013). Yes it can: On the functional abilities of the human unconscious. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 2: 195-207. DOI: 10.1177/1745691612460684.
  65. Heider F. (1958). The Psychology Oo Interpersonal Relations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  66. Herring D.R., Jabeen L.N., Hinojos M., White K.R., Taylor J.H. & Crites S.L. Jr. (2013). On the automatic activation of attitudes: A quarter century of evaluative priming. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 5: 1062-1089.
  67. Hinton G.E., McClelland J.L. & Rumelhart D. E. (1986). Distributed representations. In: Rumelhart, McClelland & the PDP Research Group, 1986, Vol. 1, pp. 77-109.
  68. Holendar D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1: 1-23. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00021269
  69. James W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover, 1950 (trad. it.: Princìpi di psicologia. Milano: Società Editrice Libraria, 1900; Milano: Principato, 1965).
  70. Jones E.E. & Harris V.A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1: 1-24. DOI: 10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0
  71. Kahneman D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux (trad. it.: Pensieri lenti e veloci. Milano: Mondadori, 2012).
  72. Kernberg O.F. (1987). The dynamic unconscious and the self. In: Stern R., editor, Theories of the Unconscious and Theories of the Self. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press, 1987, pp. 3-25.
  73. Kernberg O. (2004). Contemporary Controversies in Psychoanalytic Theory, Techniques, and their Applications. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  74. Kihlstrom J.F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 4821: 1445-1452.
  75. Kihlstrom J.F., Dorfman J. & Park L. (2007). Implicit and explicit memory and learning. In: Velmans M. & Schneider S., editors, The Blackwell Companion to Consciousness. Wiley Blackwell, 2007, pp. 525-539.
  76. Kiverstein J. (2020). Embodied cognition and the neural re use hypothesis. In: Leslie S J. & Cullen S., editors, Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science. London: Routledge, 2020, pp. 87-107.
  77. Klein D.B. (1977). The Unconscious: Invention or Discovery? A Historico-Critical Inquiry. Oxford, UK: Goodyear.
  78. Kurzban R. (2010). Why Everyone (Else) Is a Hypocrite: Evolution and the Modular Mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  79. Köveces Z. (2000). Metaphor and Emotion: Language, Culture, and Body in Human Feeling. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  80. Köveces Z. (2002). Metaphor: A Practical Introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.
  81. Lai V.T., Hagoort P. & Casasanto D. (2012). Affective primacy vs. cognitive primacy: Dissolving the debate. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 243.
  82. Lakoff G. (1987). Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press (trad. it.: Donne, fuoco e cose pericolose. Firenze: La Nuova Italia, 1999).
  83. Laws K.R., Adlington R.L., Moreno-Martinez F.J. & Gale T.M. (2010). Category-Specificity: Evidence for Modularity of Mind. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.
  84. Lazarus R.S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psychologist, 39, 2: 124-129. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.124
  85. LeDoux J.E. (1996). The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon and Schuster (trad. it.: Il cervello emotivo: alle radici delle emozioni. Milano: Baldini & Castoldi, 1998).
  86. LeDoux J.E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: A view through fear. Progress in Brain Research, 195: 431-442. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00021-0
  87. LeDoux J.E. (2015). Feelings: What are they & how does the brain make them? Daedalus, 144, 1: 96-111.
  88. Leuzinger-Bohleber M. & Kächele, editors (2015). An Open Door Review of Outcome and Process Studies in Psychoanalysis. Third Edition. London: International Psychoanalytic Association (www.ipa.world/ipa/IPA_Docs/Open%20Door%20Review%20III.pdf).
  89. Lewicki P. (1986). Nonconscious Social Information Processing. New York: Academic Press.
  90. Lilienfeld S.., Lynn, S.J. & Lohr J.M. (2015). Fifty psychological and psychiatric terms to avoid: A list of inaccurate, misleading, misused, ambiguous, and logically confused words and phrases. Frontiers in Psychology, 6: 1100.
  91. Loftus E.F. & Klinger M.R. (1992). Is the unconscious smart or dumb? American Psychologist, 47, 6: 761-765. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.6.76
  92. Maia T.V. & Cleeremans A. (2005). Consciousness: Converging insights from connectionist modeling and neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 8: 397-404.
  93. Marcel A.J. (1980). Conscious and preconscious recognition of polysemous words: Locating the selective effects of prior verbal context. In: Nickerson R.S., editor, Attention and Performance, VIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980, pp. 435-457.
  94. Marcel A.J. (1983a). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 2: 197-237. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9
  95. Marcel A.J. (1983b). Conscious and unconscious perception: An approach to the relations between phenomenal experience and perceptual processes. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 2: 238-300. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90010-5
  96. Martin A., Haxby J., Lalonde F. Wiggs C. & Ungerleider L. (1995). Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science, 270, 5233: 102-105.
  97. Martin A., Wiggs C., Ungerleider L. & Haxby J. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 6566: 649-652.
  98. Martin A., Ungerleider L. & Haxby J. (1999). Category specificity and the brain: The sensory/motor model of semantic representations of objects. In: Gazzaniga M.S., editor, The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999, pp. 1023-1036.
  99. McClelland D.C. (1961). The Achieving Society. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand Company.
  100. McClelland D.C. (1987). Human Motivation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  101. McClelland D C., Koestner R. & Weinberger J. (1989). How do self attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 4: 690-702. DOI: 10.1037/0033295X.96.4.690
  102. McClelland J.L., Rumelhart D.E. & Hinton G.E. (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing. In: Rumelhart, McClelland & the PDP Research Group, 1986, Vol. 1, pp. 3-44.
  103. McWilliams N. (2013). Psychoanalysis and research: Some reflections and opinions. Psychoanalytic Review, 100, 6: 919-931.
  104. Meier B.P. & Robinson M.D. (2006). Does “feeling down” mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 4: 451-461.
  105. Meier B.P., Moeller S.K., Riemer-Peltz M. & Robinson M.D. (2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial inferences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1: 163-174.
  106. Milner B. (2005). The medial temporal-lobe amnesic syndrome. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 28, 3: 599-611.
  107. Mitchell S.A. (1988). Relational Concepts in Psychoanalysis: An Integration. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (trad. it.: Gli orientamenti relazionali in psicoanalisi. Per un modello integrato. Torino: Bollati Boringhieri, 1993).
  108. Murphy S.T. & Zajonc R.B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5: 723-739. DOI: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.5.723
  109. Neisser U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts (trad. it.: Psicologia cognitivista. Firenze: Giunti, 1976).
  110. Newirth J. (2003). Between Emotion and Cognition: The Generative Unconscious. New York: Other Press.
  111. Niedenthal P.M., Halberstadt J.B. & Innes-Ker A.H. (1999). Emotional response categorization. Psychological Review, 106, 2: 337-361. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.337
  112. Nisbett R. & Wilson T. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychology Review, 84, 3: 231-259.
  113. Niu Y., Todd R. & Anderson A.K. (2012). Affective salience can reverse the effects of stimulus-driven salience on eye movements in complex scenes. Frontiers in Psychology, 3: 336.
  114. Norman D.A. (1986). Reflections on cognition and parallel distributed processing. In: Rumelhart, McClelland & the PDP Research Group, 1986, Vol. 2, pp. 531-546.
  115. Nummenmaa L., Hyona J. & Calvo M.G. (2010). Semantic categorization precedes affective evaluation of visual scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 2: 222-246.
  116. Pessoa L. & Adolphs R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a “low road” to “many roads” of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 11: 773-783.
  117. Pinker S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: Norton (trad. it.: Come funziona la mente. Milano: Mondadori, 2000; Roma: Castelvecchi, 2017).
  118. Pinker S. (2005). So how does the mind work? Mind and Language, 20, 1: 1-24.
  119. Popper K.R. (1934). Logik der Forschung. Zur Erkenntnistheorie der modernen Naturwissenschaft. Berlin: Springer (trad. it: Logica della scoperta scientifica. Torino: Einaudi, 1970).
  120. Posner M.I. (1973). Cognition: An introduction. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman (trad. it.: Introduzione ai processi cognitivi. Milano: FrancoAngeli, 1981).
  121. Prinz J.J. (2006). Is the mind really modular? In: Stanton R.J., editor, Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 2006, pp. 22-36
  122. Rand N. (2004). The hidden soul: The growth of the unconscious in philosophy, psychology, medicine, and literature, 1750-1900. American Imago, 61, 3: 257-289.
  123. Reber A.S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 3: 219-235. DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219
  124. Reber P.J. (2008). Cognitive neuroscience of declarative and nondeclarative memory. Advances in Psychology, 139, C: 113-125. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)10010-3
  125. Reber P.J. (2013). The neural basis of implicit learning and memory: A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia, 51, 10: 2026-2042.
  126. Ren D., Tan K., Arriaga X.B. & Chan K.Q. (2014). Sweet love: The effects of sweet taste experience on romantic perceptions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32, 7: 905-921. DOI: 10.1177/0265407514554512
  127. Riggs L., McQuiggan D.A., Anderson A.K. & Ryan J.D. (2010). Eye movement monitoring reveals differential influences of emotion on memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 1: 205.
  128. Ritter F.E., Tehranchi F. & Oury J.D. (2019). ACT R: A cognitive architecture for modeling cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10, 3: e1488.
  129. Rogers C. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In: Koch S., editor, Psychology: A Study of a Science. Vol. 3: Formulations of the Person and the Social Context. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  130. Rogers T.T. & McClelland J.L. (2014). Parallel distributed processing at 25: Further explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cognitive Science, 38, 6: 1024-1077.
  131. Rolls E.T. (2018). The Brain, Emotion, and Depression. New York: Oxford University Press.
  132. Rumelhart D.E., Hinton G.E. & McClelland J.L. (1986). A general framework for parallel distributed processing. In: Rumelhart, McClelland & the PDP Research Group, 1986, Vol. 1, pp. 45-76).
  133. Rumelhart D.E. & McClelland J.L. (1986). PDP models and general issues in cognitive science: Distributed representations. In: Rumelhart D.E., McClelland J.L. & the PDP Research Group, 1986, Vol. 1, pp. 110-146.
  134. Rumelhart D.E., McClelland J.L. & the PDP Research Group (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition: Vol. 1. Foundations; Vol. 2. Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (trad. it.: PDP, microstruttura dei processi cognitivi. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1991).
  135. Sagioglou C. & Greitemeyer T. (2014). Bitter taste causes hostility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 12: 1589-1597. DOI: 10.1177/0146167214552792
  136. Schultheiss O.C. (2013). The hormonal correlates of implicit motives. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 1: 52-65.
  137. Schultheiss O.C. & Pang J., editors (2025). Implicit Motives. New York: Oxford University Press.
  138. Schyns B. & Hansbrough T. (2008). Why the brewery ran out of beer. Social Psychology, 39, 3: 197-203. DOI: 10.1027/1864-9335.39.3.197
  139. Sedikides C., Campbell W.K., Reeder G.D. & Elliot A.J. (1998). The self-serving bias in relational context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 2: 378-386. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.378
  140. Shakow D. & Rapaport D. (1964). The Influence of Freud on American Psychology. New York: International Universities Press (Psychological Issues, 13).
  141. Sharot T. & Yonelinas A.P. (2008). Differential time-dependent effects of emotion on recollective experience and memory for contextual information. Cognition, 106, 1: 538-547.
  142. Shevrin H. & Dickman S. (1980). The psychological unconscious: A necessary assumption for all psychological theory? American Psychologist, 35, 5: 421-434. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.35.5.42
  143. Silverman L.H. (1976). Psychoanalytic theory: The reports of my death are greatly exaggerated. American Psychologist, 31, 9: 621-636. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.31.9.621
  144. Silverman L.H., Lachmann F.M & Milch R.H. (1982). The Search for Oneness. New York: International Universities Press.
  145. Silverman L.H. & Weinberger J. (1985). Mommy and I are one: Implications for psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 40, 12: 1296-1308. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.40.12.1296
  146. Smith C.P. (1992). Motivation and Personality: Handbook of Thematic Content Analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  147. Smith E.R. (1996). What do connectionism and social psychology offer each other? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 6: 893-912. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.893
  148. Squire L.R. (2009). Memory and brain systems: 1969-2009. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 41: 12711-12716. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-09.2009.
  149. Stern D.B. (1997). Unformulated Experience: From Dissociation to Imagination in Psychoanalysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press (trad. it.: L’esperienza non formulata. Dalla dissociazione all’immagine in psicoanalisi. Tirrenia [PI]: Edizioni Del Cerro, 2007).
  150. Storbeck J. & Robinson M.D. (2004). Preferences and inferences in encoding visual objects: A systematic comparison of semantic and affective priming. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1: 81-93. DOI: 10.1177/0146167203258855.
  151. Storbeck J., Robinson M.D. & McCourt M.E. (2006). Semantic processing precedes affect retrieval: The neurological case for cognitive primacy in visual processing. Review of General Psychology, 10, 1: 41-55. DOI: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.1.41
  152. Strack F. & Deutsch R. (2015). The duality of everyday life: Dual-process and dual-system models in social psychology. In: Mikulincer M. & Shaver P.R., editors, APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology. Vol. 1: Attitudes and Social Cognition. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association, 2015, pp. 891-927.
  153. Tamietto M., Castelli L., Vighetti S., Perozzo P., Geminiani G., Weiskrantz L. & de Gelder B. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 106, 42: 1761-1766.
  154. Tamietto M. & de Gelder B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 10: 697-709.
  155. Titchener E.B. (1929). Systematic Psychology. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press, 1972.
  156. Todd R.M., Cunningham W.A., Anderson A.K. & Thompson E. (2012). Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 7: 365-372.
  157. Tolman E.C. (1949). Purposive behavior in animals and men. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press (trad. it.: Il comportamento intenzionale negli animali e negli uomini. Roma: Armando, 1983).
  158. Treisman A.M. (1969). Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychological Review, 76, 3: 282-299.
  159. Tversky A. & Kahneman D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 4481: 453-458.
  160. Uleman J.S. & Bargh J.A., editors (1989). Unintended Thought. New York: Guilford.
  161. Vuilleumier P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 12: 585-594.
  162. Wallerstein R.S. (2009). What kind of research in psychoanalytic science? International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 90, 1: 109-133.
  163. Watson J.B. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: Norton (trad. it.: Il comportamentismo. Firenze: Giunti Barbera, 1983; Milano: Fabbri, 2007; Milano: Centauria, 2018).
  164. Weinberger J., Chassman E. & Delgado B. (2025). Implicit motivation: Theory, research and clinical implications. In: Schultheiss & Pang, 2025, in stampa.
  165. Weinberger J. & Levy K.N. (2005). Psychoanalysis and psychology. In: Person E.S., Cooper A.M. & Gabbard G.O., editors, Textbook of Psychoanalysis. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2005, pp. 463-478 (trad. it.: La psicologia. In: Psicoanalisi. Teoria, clinica, ricerca. Milano: Raffaello Cortina, 2006, pp. 751-775).
  166. Weinberger J. & McClelland D.C. (1990). Cognitive versus traditional motivational models: irreconcilable or complementary? In: Higgins E.T. & Sorrentino R.M., editors, Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior. New York: Guilford, 1990, Vol. 2, pp. 562-597.
  167. Weinberger J., Purcell A. & Knafo G. (2025). Affiliative motivation. In: Schultheiss & Pang, 2025, in stampa.
  168. Weinberger J. & Stoycheva B. (2020). The Unconscious: Theory, Research, and Clinical Implications. New York: Guilford.
  169. Weiner B. (1986). An Attributional Theory of Motivation and Emotion. New York: Springer.
  170. Westen D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 3: 333-371. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.333
  171. Westen D. (1999). The scientific status of unconscious processes: is Freud really dead? Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 47, 4: 1061-1106.
  172. Westen D. & Gabbard G. (2002). Developments in cognitive neuroscience: 2. Implications for theories of transference. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 50, 1: 99-134. DOI: 10.1177/00030651020500011601.
  173. Whyte L.L. (1960). The Unconscious before Freud. New York: Basic Books (trad. it.: L’inconscio prima di Freud. Una storia dell’evoluzione della coscienza umana. Roma: Astrolabio).
  174. Williams L.E. & Bargh J.A. (2008a). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 5901: 606-607.
  175. Williams L.E. & Bargh J.A. (2008b). Keeping one’s distance: The influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19, 3: 302-308.
  176. Winkielman P., Niedenthal P.M., Wielgosz J., Eelen J. & Lindner K. (2012). Embodied emotion concepts: How the brain represents emotion in the body. In: Forgas, 2000, pp. 151-175.
  177. Winter D.G. (2005). Things I’ve learned about personality from studying political leaders at a distance. Journal of Personality, 73, 3: 557-584.
  178. Yakeley J. (2018). Psychoanalysis in modern mental health practice. Lancet Psychiatry, 5, 5: 443-450. DOI: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30052-X
  179. Yoon K.L., Hong S.W., Joormann J. & Kang P. (2009). Perception of facial expressions of emotion during binocular rivalry. Emotion, 9, 2: 172-182.
  180. Zajonc R.B. (2000). Feeling and thinking: Closing the debate over the independence of affect. In: Forgas, 2000, pp. 31-58.
  181. Zhong C.B. & Leonardelli G.J. (2008). Cold and lonely: Does social exclusion literally feel cold? Psychological Science, 19, 9: 838-842.

Joel Weinberger, L’inconscio. Perché ci è voluto così tanto tempo, cosa sappiamo oggi, e alcune implicazioni cliniche in "PSICOTERAPIA E SCIENZE UMANE" 3/2025, pp 377-410, DOI: 10.3280/PU2025-003001